I do wonder whether the Trademark Office is influenced by
(and if not, should be influenced by) empirical evidence concerning how
consumers actually perceive marks instead of completely relying on the judicial
interpretation of consumer psychology (read the U.S. Supreme Court’s Qualitex and Wal-Mart decisions) that has pervaded U.S. trademark law, for
example with the Spectrum of Distinctiveness inquiry. (Of course, we do use surveys to test
consumer reaction to a mark). Could that
be part of the reason that the Trademark Office is increasing its discussions
of “aesthetic functionality, failure to function as a mark, inherent
distinctiveness, acquired distinctiveness, and non-reputation based competitive
advantage?” Could it be that the
Trademark Office is utilizing the doctrinal tools that it has to correct for
problems with our traditional tests for trademark protection, including the
Spectrum of Distinctiveness? (probably
not, but maybe it should be) Ordinarily,
a threshold issue for a court is whether the purported trademark owner has a
mark that is functioning as a trademark—the question is whether the mark
is distinctive. In a 2009 article in the
Arizona State Law Journal, Thomas R. Lee, Eric D. DeRosia, and Glen L.
Christensen, address the merits of the Spectrum of Distinctiveness, in An
Empirical and Consumer Psychology Analysis of Trademark Distinctiveness.
The authors state that:
The evaluation of consumer perception is hardly a
matter of judicial competence. Instead, like many of the key issues in
trademark law, questions of distinctiveness are matters that can and should be
informed by the theoretical and empirical tools employed by those who undertake
a careful, scholarly study of such issues. . . . Questions
of distinctiveness, in particular, lend themselves to rigorous theoretical
analysis using tools developed in the scholarly study of consumer psychology.
Such questions are not merely theoretical; they lend themselves to empirical
study using accepted methods of quantitative measurement.
That is the enterprise of this article. After first setting forth some
additional background on the law's classification of word marks (in Part II),
we consider the justifications offered in the scholarly literature for the law
in this area (in Part III). Finding those justifications analytically
unsatisfying and largely question-begging, we marshal theoretical tools from
the field of consumer psychology (in Part IV) that provide important insights
into the viability of the law's word mark taxonomy. Using these tools, we show
that the law's longstanding assumptions about consumer perception of word marks
are highly questionable. Specifically, we employ consumer psychology models of
the way consumers perceive visual information
to hypothesize that when identifying a product's source, consumers will rely
primarily on non-linguistic visual cues (e.g., the location and size of the
mark as displayed on a product package) rather than the mark's semantic
meaning.
We then present a series of empirical studies of these
assumptions (in Part V). Our empirical investigation seeks to measure word mark
distinctiveness in the relevant context of trademark use. Using a series of
stimuli representative of consumer packaged goods, we hold constant the various
contextual indicators of meaning (such as prominence and placement of the word
mark on the package) while varying the word marks that appear in the context of
the package. This methodology allows us to measure the impact of the
classification of a particular word mark--as generic, descriptive, suggestive,
arbitrary, or fanciful--on trademark distinctiveness. Our study thus allows us
to offer an empirical answer to the trademark distinctiveness question long
determined by judicial intuition and precedent. Our empirical findings
thoroughly undercut the word mark taxonomy that has long dictated the coverage
of the Lanham Act. We demonstrate that descriptive marks are no less source-indicating than suggestive,
arbitrary, and fanciful marks. We also show that it is the non-lexical cues of
typical trademark use (e.g., the location and size of the mark) that cause
consumers to find descriptive marks to be source-indicating.
Finally (in Part VI), we offer some observations
about the implications of our findings for the law's treatment of word marks.
Our principal conclusion is that the law's treatment of “merely descriptive”
marks cannot be justified on the basis of its faulty premises of
distinctiveness. The main implication of that conclusion is a proposal to
jettison legal tests that are based on those premises. Thus, we advocate the
abandonment of the Abercrombie dichotomy between “inherently distinctive” and
“non-inherently distinctive” word marks, eliminating (at the threshold protectability
stage) the longstanding categories of descriptive, suggestive, arbitrary, and
fanciful trademarks. Under our approach, all non-generic word marks would be
eligible for protection so long as they satisfy the “trademark use”
requirement. . . .
The approach we advocate is admittedly heretical, as it calls
for a substantial overhaul of a system of classification that has long stood as
an unquestioned fixture of trademark law. But if that fixture can be shown to
be indefensible on theoretical and empirical grounds, it deserves to be
questioned. Once “merely descriptive” words are seen as distinctive
source-indicators when used in a typical “trademark use” context, they should
not be foreclosed from protection on the basis of erroneous assumptions about
consumer psychology.
Heretical indeed!
What do you think?
1 comment:
Without having read the article, I think that looking at packaging addresses only a small subset of ways in which we encounter marks and so is of limited usefulness. Of course we all say to ourselves "ah, those big fancy letters at the top of the blister pack must be the trademark," but we also need to think about it in oral environments, when in the body of text, Twitter handles, Google keywords, etc. This test overrates the distinctiveness of descriptive terms because of added information, i.e., placement, but that information simply isn't there in many encounters with a trademark. The Abercrombie spectrum has been used for 50 years and generally does the job pretty well.
Post a Comment